Is ocean acidification driving fish bonkers?

The long and winding road of replication

Like Comment
Read the paper

The results were clear and consistent. They were easily the most profound findings that I had seen in my 20-year career in science. I am referring to the weird behavioural responses of coral reef fishes after being exposed to a few days of seawater CO2 levels (~1,000 µatm) representing end-of-century ocean acidification (cf. current-day levels of ~450 µatm)1-3. Despite decades of data from physiologists showing that fish are well-equipped to regulate their internal acid-base balance even under extreme water CO2 levels (e.g., 15,000 µatm)4, these new studies by an Australian research group on the Great Barrier Reef were showing that much lower CO2 levels (~1,000 µatm) caused dramatic behavioural and sensory impairments in a suite of coral reef fishes.

 

Perhaps the most alarming of these impairments was that fish exposed to end-of-century CO2 levels became highly attracted to the chemical cues of their predators, rather than strongly avoiding them like fish under current-day CO2 levels1,3. The potential ramifications of these findings were frightening given that our oceans continue to absorb human-derived CO2 from the atmosphere.

 

What were the physiological mechanisms underlying these behavioural impairments? A comparative physiologist by training, this question floated through my mind as I established a new research lab upon relocating to Townsville in north-eastern Australia in 2011. In 2014, the opportunity arose to start addressing this question with colleagues during a fieldtrip to the Lizard Island Research Station in the northern Great Barrier Reef, where many of the pioneering behavioural studies had been conducted. The first step was to replicate the behavioural impairments described in the pioneering studies so we had a fish model system on which to base our physiological experiments. Fast-forward two years and we still had not succeeded at this first step, despite additional attempts in Townsville in 2015 and another fieldtrip to Lizard Island in 2016. We had tested fish responses to predator chemical cues, spontaneous activity levels, behavioural lateralisation (preference for turning left or right), and an aspect of visual acuity, all of which had previously been reported to be impaired by elevated CO2. We even used species and life-stages previously shown to suffer major behavioural impairments. It became clear to us from our multi-year, multi-species dataset that end-of-century CO2 levels do not impair the behaviour of coral reef fishes.

 

Figure 2 in Clark et al. (2020)
Percentage of time (mean ± s.e.m.) that fishes from early life stages and later life stages (including mid and late life stages) spent on one side of a two-current choice flume during experiments at the Lizard Island Research Station in 2016. Species are (a, b) Acanthochromis polyacanthus, (c) Dascyllus aruanus, (d, e) Dischistodus perspicillatus. Control fish (maintained at ambient reef CO2 of approximately 520 μatm) in closed grey circles, CO2-exposed fish (maintained at around 1,090 μatm) in open blue circles. Fish were given 40 min to habituate to the flume. Predator chemical cues (Cephalopholis cyanostigma) were then introduced to one side of the flume for 20 min and then switched to the other side for another 20 min. n of biologically independent animals are shown in the figure panels. Fish illustrations by E. Walsh.


What were we to do now? We had a large dataset full of null results. Should we file these away and contribute to the “file drawer” effect that plagues many scientific disciplines? Thankfully, from the outset of our experiments in 2014, we realised we needed to be as rigorous, transparent and objective as possible to avoid the various experimenter biases that can influence observational experiments5. We knew our science was robust, and we had full confidence in our results. We also felt a responsibility to inform the global scientific community that we were unable to replicate dramatic effects published in prestigious journals such as Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Ecology Letters, and Nature Climate Change. We greatly appreciate that Nature has been a leader on the topic of reproducibility, and that the journal recognised the important contribution our study made to the field6.

 

So, the elephant remains patiently sitting in the room... How is it possible that our results are so dramatically different from a decade of publications by another research group? We cannot provide a satisfactory answer to that question at the moment, but we hope that our research stimulates thoughtful discussion and reflection among interested readers in the scientific community.

 


Thanks for comments on this post from my co-authors Graham Raby, Dominique Roche, Sandra Binning, Ben Speers-Roesch, Fredrik Jutfelt and Josefin Sundin. Fish photo by Fredrik Jutfelt.

References

1            Dixson, D. L., Munday, P. L. & Jones, G. P. Ocean acidification disrupts the innate ability of fish to detect predator olfactory cues. Ecology Letters 13, 68-75, doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01400.x (2010).

2            Munday, P. L. et al. Ocean acidification impairs olfactory discrimination and homing ability of a marine fish. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 1848-1852, doi:10.1073/pnas.0809996106 (2009).

3            Munday, P. L. et al. Replenishment of fish populations is threatened by ocean acidification. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 12930-12934, doi:10.1073/pnas.1004519107 (2010).

4            Ishimatsu, A., Hayashi, M., Lee, K.-S., Kikkawa, T. & Kita, J. Physiological effects on fishes in a high-CO2 world. Journal of Geophysical Research: Oceans 110, C09S09, doi:10.1029/2004JC002564 (2005).

5            Ihle, M., Winney, I. S., Krystalli, A. & Croucher, M. Striving for transparent and credible research: practical guidelines for behavioral ecologists. Behavioral Ecology 28, 348-354, doi:10.1093/beheco/arx003 (2017).

6            Clark, T. D. et al. Ocean acidification does not impair the behaviour of coral reef fishes. Nature 577, 370-375, doi:10.1038/s41586-019-1903-y (2020).

 

Go to the profile of Timothy Clark

Timothy Clark

Associate Professor, Deakin University

No comments yet.